Connecting the Multiple Voices of the Pattern that Connects (Part #2)
[Parts: First | Prev | Next | Last | All] [Links: To-K | Refs ]
In music, an octave or perfect octave (sometimes called the diapason) is a series of eight notes occupying the interval between (and including) two notes, one having twice the frequency of vibration of the other. The octave relationship is a natural phenomenon that has been referred to as the "basic miracle of music", the use of which is "common in most musical systems". The interval between the first and second harmonics of the harmonic series is an octave. In Western music notation, notes separated by an octave (or multiple octaves) have the same name and are of the same pitch class.
The challenge in ordering complexity in the light of such an array is evident from the following:
In making any such choice the question is the capacity to discern distinctions, rather than conflating (or confusing) the possibilities -- possibly deliberately -- with the respective strategic consequences. Both choices evoke contrasting uses of metaphor to facilitate comprehension. For "lower octaves" a metaphor may be required to frame implication and the "richness" associated with each element. For "higher octaves" a metaphor may be required to suggest the relation to other elements, otherwise easily ignored. Both forms of metaphor may be questionable. Memory "management" in relation to complexity is a focus of research on "chunking" -- a process by which small individual pieces of a set of information are bound together to create a meaningful whole in memory.
Sound and colour: Verbal discussion of discernment capacity may be less meaningful than through the manner in which distinctions of sound or colour are made. In both cases a very wide variety of distinctions can potentially be recognized. A key question is whether and how a distinction can be said to have been discerned if it cannot be named. Of relevance to this argument is the number which can be unambiguously named -- with the associated cognitive, communication and strategic implications:
Taste and smell: Similar points could be made with regard to the discernment of tastes -- as cultivated by those with "educated palates" with respect to wines (How can you tell if someone has an educated palate in wine tasting? Quora). Related insights are offered by odours -- as cultivated by those with an "educated nose" (Rachel Sarah Herz, The role of odor-evoked memory in psychological and physiological health, Brain Sciences, 6, 2016, 3; The Value of an Educated Nose, Vinesse, 26 September 2013). Recent research has shown that humans can identify more than 1 trillion smells -- although how many can be named is another matter.
The matter can also be explored through the number of people a person can "know", whatever this is variously held to mean. How "well" is another known? One upper limit for stable relationships is suggested by Dunbar's number of 150, although it is less evident how this may vary across cultures and comprehension of "knowing". A distinction in terms of "octaves" could be considered for mnemonic purposes.
Memory span: Another approach could be through exploration of the size of sets considered memorable in practice by those engaged with any such set -- as with species, music, books, celebrities, etc.How many "things" can be remembered and successfully communicated -- despite the challenge of aging (Lorea Lastiri, Does Your Brain Have a Memory Limit? Iris, 10 February 2023). More pertinent is the relevance to collective memory (Societal Learning and the Erosion of Collective Memory, 1980).
Such examples reframe the question of how values and strategic objectives are discerned and articulated. There is little consensus regarding discernment capacity in the examples indicated. The confusion is especially evident with respect to the pattern of tones. Despite widespread familiarity with music, supported by extensive research, this is most evident in contrasting appreciation of tuning systems. Of relevance is the challenge of the "colour blind" -- comparable to that of the "tone deaf" -- both with their global strategic implications (Group of 7 Dwarfs: Future-blind and Warning-deaf, 2018).
Set "completeness": The various examples frame the question as to whether a set is "complete" in any systemic sense, possibly to be understood as "well-formed" -- and how this may or may not be the case (as discussed below, for convenience). Issues in that respect include:
Comprehensibility and memorability: In contrast to the preoccupation of mathematics and other disciplines, the approach adopted here is to focus on comprehensibility and memorability, guided by the apparent number preferences in any articulation, as previously explored (Patterns of N-foldness: Comparison of integrated multi-set concept schemes as forms of presentation, 1980). The approach is consistent with the preoccupations of general systems research and the recognition of correspondences ( Theories of Correspondences -- and potential equivalences between them in correlative thinking, 2007; Correspondences between Traditional Constellations and Pattern Languages, 2014).
Of particular concern in what follows is recognition of any insight -- from the articulation of disparate sets -- into the nature of transformations within the pattern constituted by a set. Such insights may take a variety of forms between which it is assumed that a degree of correspondence may potentially be found -- if only understood in mnemonic and metaphorical terms. The transformations explored in most detail seem to be those between polyhedra and those associated with the Tonnetz. From a general systems perspective, it is assumed that the sense of "octave" suggests a degree of correspondence between sets of different degrees of articulation.
It is intriguing to note the psychosocial dynamics associated with the articulation of relatively complex sets in a particular domain. The dynamics are perhaps best illustrated by the history of the evolution of the periodic table of chemical elements (D. Michael P. Mingos, The Periodic Table I: historical development and essential features, 2020; Geoff Rayner-Canham, The Periodic Table: Past, Present, and Future, 2020). Equivalent "struggles" and disputes over how elements should be grouped and organized are remarkably illustrated by the many efforts to represent the "table" otherwise -- and more appropriately (Types of Periodic Table, Wikipedia; Mark R. Leach, The INTERNET Database of Periodic Tables). Corresponding initiatives are evident with respect to the array of tones, as noted below -- complicated by contrasting capacities to distinguish between them according to particular conventions and preferences.
[Parts: First | Prev | Next | Last | All] [Links: To-K | Refs ]